I am living through a unique experiment
Few photographs drew so much attention in biology, as
one showing two female langur-monkeys chasing a male (click here).
Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, who
took the picture, is a primatologist and a feminist. Two of her books are The
Woman that never evolved (1981) and Mother Nature (1999). The
following interview took place in Tucson, June 1997.
Why did you study the langurs?
In the sixties I was very much interested in the
effects of population-density on human behaviour. John Calhoun had just done
his famous study on Norway rats. He allowed them to multiply, and he let them
breed and breed until they were all crowded together, and then he began to see
what he called 'social pathology'. I couldn't study this for humans, but I
heard about these langurs, so I went to India to study infanticide in langurs
because I thought it was being produced by crowding.
The first thing I found was that males in
Langur-troops are usually quite tolerant of infants, instead of being
infanticidal. Young infants come up and jump on the male as if he was a trampoline.
They hang on his tail and swing on his tail when he is up in a tree, when he is
mating with their mother the older juveniles will bat at him and harass him. And
this is very annoying behavior, I think you have to call such a male tolerant.
He might get fed up and threaten them, but he doesn't kill them.
What kind of monkeys are Langurs?
Langurs are colobines. The subfamily Colobinae is very
widespread; you find them in Africa, as the black and white monkeys, all over
India as Hanuman langurs and in South-east Asia as leaf eating monkeys and
those wonderful droopy nosed proboscis monkeys in Borneo. By far the most
common of the colobines, the most terrestrial, the easiest to study and best
studied, is the Hanuman-Langur. They are named for Hanuman, the monkey-God, the
loyal servant to king Rama in the Hindu epic, the Ramayana. Hanuman langurs
tend to live in one-male groups, though you can sometimes find them in
multi-male groups. So you have one male with six females to twelve or more
females, up to fifty-nine females in the largest one-male group I know of.
These are breeding units. Females stay in the same home-range for their entire
lives, among their mothers, grand-mothers, sisters, daughters and aunts. Territories
are passed down from mother to daughter. These home-ranges slightly overlap,
and when females meet at the borders of their ranges you have territorial
aggression, involving both the male travelling with the group and the
females. The females are actually much more persistent in territory defence,
and often quite aggressive. Females in intergroup encounters are extremely
active, and it makes sense. They are defending the resources that are available
to them to survive and reproduce. There is a photograph of two Langur-groups
fighting one another, and the male is just sitting there as comfortable as can
be, while the females are skirmishing at the border, lined up at one another
and slapping.
Males either leave the group at maturity, or more
often what happens is that they are driven out by another male. Immediately
after a father and his sons are ousted from a troop, they travel together for a
while, but they usually join with other males. There is extremely little
aggression among these males, you think you could not hold all these males
together, that they would not get along. In fact, you cannot even detect in an
all-male band a dominance hierarchy.
So males are travelling together in all-male bands,
there may be anywhere between two to sixteen males, these are fairly flexible
groupings. These males roam around and cover tremendous distances, and they are
very hard to study for this reason.
In the morning when the resident male in a group of
females wakes up, he goes to the top of a tree, and he gives these
long-distance shouts: whoop, whoop! It is like: "I'm here, don't even
bother trying today!". And the all-male band whoops back. And you think:
Why are they letting him know that they are trespassing in his territory? Why
are they giving him this information about where they are? Basically I think
they are saying: Do it again, let's hear a little more. I think it is
information-gathering, and it is in the resident male's interest to answer,
because if he doesn't, they are more likely to come. But they may come anyway.
They are constantly probing and exploring.
But the resident male is trying to chase them away.
The resident male tries to keep them off. There is a
reproductive advantage to being a resident male, -he has a disproportionate
probability of siring the offspring-, and so there is intense competition to be
that male. And the males in the all-male band will attempt to take over and to
oust the resident male.
When the males that come in actually attack the
resident male, there is a big chance that he will fight back, and one of them
will be injured. In such a climate injury is a serious problem; it could lead
to gangrene and an animal could die. So neither wants to inflict damage on the
other that would then provoke a response of serious damage. They are held in
check, rather like nuclear powers, by the threat of retaliation. But if there
is a skewing of the odds, such that the resident male is weak, or there are so
many males in the all-male band, the male band is sometimes able to drive out
the resident male, and all the young males may follow later, or they all go at
once.
It is a very chaotic time. Everyone is fighting with
everyone else, in the sense that the females are still perhaps exhibiting
antagonistic behaviour toward these outsiders, and the males themselves are
still fighting. And this is one of the reasons why people said that infanticide
is just an accident, the infants are killed incidentally during this chaos. It
is very hard to collect information when this is going on.
When these new males come into a troop, previously
there were no dominance differences perceptible among them. After they take
over a troop, -and now you might have five adult males resident in the troop-,
the competitive behaviour to construct a dominance hierarchy begins to emerge,
and one of these males in general emerges as the new alpha male. Occasionally
two or three males will stay in for a time, and you will have briefly a
multi-male group.
Are they brothers?
We don't know. We never obtained DNA, and it is
unlikely that we will get it for India because these are sacred monkeys, and it
is difficult for Americans to collect these data; we had a number of political
problems working in India. What may be the case though, is that if the ousted
male is still very powerful, it might make sense to tolerate some competition.
You'll share some copulations in order to have the alliance of males.
You took a picture that became quite famous; Two female langurs chasing a male who has just captured an infant. What actually happened?
It is a very gripping image of a male who has an
infant in his mouth. The image is blurred, you can just see the tail of the
infant, its body flying off in space. And two females are charging the male to
try to get that infant back.
The fascinating part is that neither female is the
infant's mother. They are both older females in the group. The one in the
forefront, a female called Sol, was almost certainly a close relative of the
infant, perhaps a grandmother, perhaps a great aunt. We estimate that females
in langur groups on average are related some place on the order of 1/16 of
their genes by common descent, like first or second cousins. This was a female
who did not reproduce during all the years of the study where she was observed.
Whether one wants to call her menopausal or not, she was for all practical
purposes post-reproductive. I am assuming that both are close relatives and
therefore intervening and trying to save the infant.
What about the mother of the infant?
The mother of the infant herself was a young female at
the peak of her reproductive career. Much more cautious, sitting on the
sideline, letting the older females protect the infant.
In fact there are other cases where a mother for
example allows her infant to drop from a tree, the male will rush to it, and it
is not the mother who goes down to save it, it is again these older females.
Why these older females instead of the mother?
What I believe is going on is that females with a
lower reproductive value have a different cost-benefit ratio for taking
chances on behalf of relatives. This can be seen in other contexts. So for
example McCarthy and Bugos, when they collected data on maternal infanticide
among the Ayoreo Indians in South America, found that very young mothers and
mothers with high reproductive value were much more likely to give up, and not
go on with an infant under bad circumstances. Whereas an older mother will go
ahead, no matter what, because she is getting near the end of her reproductive
opportunities. So in other words, females are making very different decisions
based on where they are in their life-history, and this is an important point
when we come at a problem with pre-conceptions about what is maternal
behaviour. Biologically, what is a good mother? Well, the truth is there is no
one good mother, no one good solution. As post-enlightment Western human
beings we have very set ideas about what is a good mother. But in biological
terms, maternal investment is very facultative.
Why is the male trying to kill the infant?
As one male emerges as the new troop-leader, he will
start to stalk females with young infants, and disproportionably the targets of
his assaults are six months and under. Before a male is ousted, a langur male
is extraordinarily tolerant of offspring, many of whom are likely to be his
own. It is only when males enter the troop from outside the breeding-system,
and encounter females that they have not mated with, that they exhibit this
aggressive behaviour.
The male's behaviour is very goal-directed. He will
stalk an infant for hours or days. He looks in different ways, in every
direction except at the mother-infant pair, pretending not to be interested
while moving closer. Then suddenly he grabs towards them. The behavior of the
females also tells you something.
Females without infants don't particularly avoid this
male. Females with young infants are moving away. They are very, very skittish
of him. The fights that happen after the male has taken over, between females
and the male, tend to be when the male has attacked someone's infant. And other
females then come back and fight. In other words it does not appear to be a
male attacking females in an effort to hurt the females. Rather the females
attack the male because he is trying to hurt an infant. When you are watching
this, no-one who has ever seen this happen has ever come away questioning the
idea that this is goal-directed behaviour on the part of the male, it is not
some incidental byproduct of social disturbance or chaos.
The sexual selection hypothesis that I proposed is
based on Darwin's theory of sexual selection. You have competition between
males for access to females, and in some cases female choice. Males are in
competition with other males to breed with females. If a male has taken over a
group of females, he has only a brief window of opportunity, twenty-seven
months on average. If a female weans the offspring she already has, if she
continues to lactate, she will not ovulate again for perhaps a year. By that
time the male could be driven out by another male. So he is basically trying to
subvert female choice. By eliminating the offspring of the last male who sired
her infant, the new male creates a circumstance whereby the female is going to
be under pressure to ovulate again soon.
Why don't females simply refuse to mate with an
infanticidal male?
Because females are in competition with other females
for representation in the next generation. If a female refuses to mate with an
infanticidal male, and therefore waits, say two years before she breeds again,
she leaves fewer progeny in the next generation then will her sister who does
go ahead and breed. The second point is that if she is in a population where
male take-overs are happening, her son, once grown up, will be at a
disadvantage in competition with other males if he is not infanticidal. So
females are under tremendous pressure to make the best of this appalling situation
by breeding with an infanticidal male.
How do females defend their young?
The most obvious thing of course is that they try to
stay clear of the male. Mothers threatened by infanticide are much more
restrictive of their infants.
Females with an infant may leave the group and travel
apart. If she has an infant approaching the age of weaning she may try to leave
that troop with the male and the all-male band. Furthermore, females can sort
of pre-emptively reduce the probability of infanticide if they have bred with
a male before he comes into the troop. It is the mother who is the cue to
whether or not he attacks or tolerates the infant. I have seen females kidnap
infants from other groups and bring them back to their own, but so long as the
female is familiar, the male does not attack these little strangers. So I
hypothesized that females solicit outside males in addition to her resident
male as a defensive strategy in case one of these comes in. In this way she
manipulates information available to males about paternity.
Why was your interpretation of infanticide in langurs
so controversial?
Part of the controversy that has come up is that
people have assumed that I am talking about 'a gene for infanticide', a male
either has it or he doesn't. I have never said anything about it, -I don't
know. The same male is infanticidal in one context and not in another. And I
know that some strains of mice are more infanticidal then others, so we have
good reason to think that this is something that is inherited. But even in an
infanticidal strain of mice, such a male may only engage in infanticidal
behaviour 70% of the time when he has an opportunity to do it; so clearly
context matters. The fact that the mechanism is still unknown was one problem.
But the other, larger issue, was a reluctance to accept that something so
obviously detrimental to the species or the group could be adaptive for
individual males.
When my langur research was published, some people
drew a parallel with human child abuse and the stepfather-phenomenon. It is
structurally similar, but the rationale for it I now believe to be quite
different. A human male who kills his stepchildren is usually not enhancing his
reproductive access by that female. I agree completely with Martin Daly and
Margo Wilson that the issue here is best explained in terms of competition for
resources. The stepfather who assaults an infant belonging to his mate's previous
partner is responding to the demands that an infant is making on him, on the
mother, and on the household-resources.
Infants of course have been selected to be basically
insatiable in their demands, and you have a situation that is more nearly
comparable to another kind of langur behavior. Langurs are infant-sharers;
females take and carry other females' infants. The only time you see infants
abused by female langurs is when a female takes an infant and is subsequently
no longer motivated to carry it. A female who has taken an infant that is not
her own, and grows tired of holding it and no other female comes up to take it
from her; -she tries to push it off herself. For an infant not being held by a
female is paramount to death! If it is on its own, it is subject to predation.
Infants are selected to cling like glue to whoever has them. So you have an
allomother, -a female other than the mother-, pushing the baby off, the infant
trying to stay on, and this is when you see langur females sitting on the baby,
pushing it against a rock, this kind of abuse. This is much more relevant to
what is going on in human child-abuse, than is infanticide by adult male
langurs. It is a stepfather who is not motivated to invest as much in the
infant as the infant is asking for. He is pushing it off, and that is when he
is losing his temper and inflicts mortal damage on the infant.
Why is it the females that stay in their natal group,
instead of the males?
Most Old World monkeys, as among most mammals, if they
are social, are matrilocal. The advantages to a female of having her relatives
around are very great.
It is usually males that disperse because females,
-probably to avoid inbreeding- exhibit a preference for mating with novel
males. So that a male who remains in his natal troop is at a
breeding-disadvantage with other males, because he is often not the preferred
male. I believe that he moves to a new troop to advance his breeding-options.
The other possibility is that he himself is leaving to avoid inbreeding.
There are however circumstances when males can't
afford to move, as when they are threatened by male alliances in competing
groups. When fathers, brothers and sons remain together, as among chimpanzees,
it can force females to move instead. But it is very costly to a female to lose
the social support of her relatives. If her mother's feeding grounds are
particularly good, an advantaged daughter may refuse to move as Jane Goodall
has reported for Flo's matriline at Gombe. Once patrilocality emerges though,
females are at a real disadvantage. Control of feeding grounds and resources
fall increasingly under patrilineal control.
The key phrase here is being able to control the
resources. If parents in a patrilocal situation are aware that their sons are
going to have allies around, while their daughters will not, they risk
territory and resources passed on to daughters being diverted into her mate or
husband's patri-line. So parents bias toward sons in intergenerational
transmission, because their sons are better able to protect and maintain it. I
am obviously thinking here of human examples.
Why are a few primate-species patrilocal?
Yes, why do males sometimes stay? Why are there male
patrilocal groups? Well, lets think about where we find them. We find them in
species like chimpanzees. Chimp males have a special problem, because they are
in competition with the males in competing communities, males who will come in
and if they have the chance wipe them out and take their females. How can they
protect the resources they have, which consist of the females in it? They need
alliances with other males, and the best allies are relatives. You can depend
on them the best, a relative has a different cost-benefit ratio for helping
you versus competing with you than does a strange male. So males stay with
their fathers and grandfathers and brothers in order to protect themselves,
the females they want access to, and the offspring they produce, from other
males. The males won't leave because if a male goes off he'll be killed by the
males of competing communities. So the female leaves, and as Anne Pusey suggests,
she uses her sexual receptivity as a passport to enter new communities. She
tries to settle there, she may lose her first infant, it may be killed by the
resident males, but her later offspring will probably get to survive.
Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa has noted that there is a bias in terms of males attacking sons in chimpanzees. It is one of the few cases outside of humans where you have sex-biased infanticide. The sample-sizes are still quite small, I think it is twelve cases of infanticide in chimps, and nine of the twelve are males rather than females, killed by males. And you have to think, why on earth is this going on? I think that the males who control the community, this brotherhood of males, have much more to lose by allowing another male's son to grow up in their community. A female if she grows up in their community, might leave, and go off and breed elsewhere, or they might have a chance to breed with her. But if a male that is not related to them grows up and joins the brotherhood, they will be sharing the resources of their community, the females in it, and sharing sexual access with this unrelated male. And this is a tremendous dent in the inclusive fitness of these males, to tolerate an alien male among them.
What is your opinion about the idea that in some
distant human past, females were dominant over males?
I think it is feminist invention, a psychological
antidote against myths of male dominance. It is an effort to invent an
alternate reality. But the archaeological or ethnographic evidence is just not
there to support it. For instance fertility figures, they say look as if
people worshipped females, so females must have ruled. But all this proves is
that somebody in those societies was fascinated by female fecundity. These statuettes
tell us nothing about political power.
Hunter-gatherers are often held up as being egalitarian,
but according to my reading of hunter-gathering monographs, -and I read quite a
few of them-, they are more egalitarian then most, but even so males are
dominant. I don't find even among hunter-gatherers a very convincing case where
females are dominant or even completely equal.
In the way I see the world today, as a Western woman living in the late twentieth century, I am living through a unique and really wonderful experiment for females. I have access to education, I own property in my own name, I have a degree of self-determination, to be able to marry and reproduce and still be able to pursue a career, sustain an independent life intellectually. This is unheard of for the genus Homo, utterly novel. What tends to be overlooked in some feminist mythologies about how ‘we are regaining a matriarchal past’, is that this is an illusion, and it is dangerous. I think we need to understand that this is an experiment, and that it is fragile. Rights that so many now take for granted, no one familiar with our evolutionary history would sensibly take for granted.